You are viewing the site in preview mode

Skip to main content

The relationship between implicit motives and physical activity: a scoping review

Abstract

Background

Interventions that leverage implicit motives — affect-based, non-conscious motivational dispositions — may increase physical activity by making physical activity more pleasurable. However, there is no evidence synthesis of the empirical data linking the major implicit motives (achievement, affiliation, and power motives) and physical activity. We aimed to close this research gap.

Methods

Following a systematic literature search in the PsycInfo, PubMed, and Web of Science databases until August 2024, we performed a scoping review. We included German- or English-language publications in peer-reviewed journals or books that followed an observational or intervention study design. Studies had to link ≥ 1 major implicit motive measured via a well-established method to physical activity behavior. We critically appraised the methodological quality of the included studies using an adaptation of the JBI critical appraisal checklist for analytical cross-sectional studies and synthesized the evidence qualitatively.

Results

Out of 1047 potentially relevant records, five publications (seven studies, N = 550) were included. All eligible studies were observational (six cross-sectional, one prospective longitudinal). The achievement motive was researched the most. The data indicated a relatively consistent positive association between physical activity and the achievement motive, particularly in athletes and in sports-specific settings. The associations with the affiliation and power motives were more mixed. Most studies were conducted in sports-specific settings. All studies elicited methodological concerns, to varying degrees.

Conclusions

The available data indicate a positive association between achievement motive strength and physical activity. However, important limitations, especially the lack of intervention studies and the use of non-gold standard assessment methods, limit the confidence in the findings. More, methodologically sound research is needed to better understand the link between implicit motives and physical activity, especially in the general population.

Systematic review registration

PROSPERO CRD42023392198.

Peer Review reports

Background

Physical inactivity is widespread. The results of a recent meta-analysis indicate that only about 17% of adults meet combined aerobic and muscle-strengthening physical activity guidelines [1]. This high level of physical inactivity not only harms people’s individual health by increasing the risk of diseases like cardiovascular disease and various cancers [2,3,4] but also causes dramatic costs to healthcare systems [5,6,7]. Thus, designing and implementing effective physical activity promotion interventions is of utmost importance to counteract the negative consequences of physical inactivity. In order to design effective interventions and increase physical activity, knowledge of the causal determinants of physical activity behavior is necessary. Implicit motives might be a causal determinant of physical activity.

Implicit motives are non-conscious and affect-based motivational dispositions that lead to individuals experiencing certain activities and contexts as rewarding (i.e., incentives) or aversive (i.e., disincentives), respectively [8]. The most commonly studied implicit motives are the needs for achievement, affiliation, and power [8]. Individuals with a strong achievement motive derive satisfaction from autonomously mastering challenging tasks [8]. Those with a pronounced affiliation motive find establishing, maintaining, or restoring positive interpersonal relationships rewarding [8]. Those with a strong power motive derive satisfaction from having impact (e.g., physical, emotional) on others [8]. Implicit motives have remarkable predictive validity for activities ranging from everyday choices and preferences (e.g., a preference for feedback while engaging with challenging tasks by achievement-motivated persons) to political action (e.g., prediction of peaceful resolution of crises by the affiliation motive or initiation of armed conflicts by the power motive) [8].

People differ in their implicit motives, which can be understood as a part of human personality [9]. Implicit motives appear to be conceptually and empirically different from related concepts such as explicit (i.e., conscious) motives [8, 10] or the Big-Five personality traits [11].Footnote 1

Their hedonic basis is why implicit motives might be a causal determinant of physical activity behavior: If a person’s implicit motives and the incentives provided by the context of being physically active align (i.e., a motive-incentive-fit exists) and, thus, the person can successfully attain motive-specific incentives (i.e., the implicit motives get satisfied), positive affect should be the consequence [13]. Persons with stronger implicit motives should accordingly also have a greater capacity to derive positive affect from the satisfaction of their implicit motives [8, 13]. At this stage, the concepts of hedonism [14] and motive-driven learning [13] become relevant: Through their effects on affect, implicit motives play a key role in the learning of stimuli, behaviors, and contexts that are related to the affective experience [13]. In the context of physical activity, this could mean that the positive affect resulting from encountering a motive-incentive fit and attaining motive-specific incentives would contribute to learning that this physical activity context is one of reward [13]. Simply put, the positive affect resulting from the respective physical activity behavior/context should serve as a “motivating force,” thereby increasing the probability of re-engaging in that physical activity behavior again in the future. Figure 1 summarizes this proposed mechanism.

Fig. 1
figure 1

The proposed mechanism of how implicit motives might causally influence physical activity behavior. Note: If the incentives provided by a physical activity context fit a person’s implicit motives, incentive attainment is facilitated. Incentive attainment, in turn, leads to positive affect. Experiencing positive affect in the context of being physically active should make the overall physical activity experience pleasurable, thereby increasing the likelihood of engaging in that physical activity behavior again in the future. Thus, in essence, if a person’s implicit motives can be satisfied by a given physical activity context, the physical activity should be perceived as more affectively rewarding and pleasurable, thus motivating future physical activity in this context. Figure available at https://osf.io/8jyhg/ [12], under a CC-BY4.0 license

For example, highly achievement-motivated persons might enjoy a physical activity context which allows them to get better at a certain physical activity, and thus beneficially respond to environments where they are provided with personalized feedback [10]. Highly affiliation-motivated individuals might be particularly responsive to physical activity contexts that include familiar social environments and cooperation [10]. Particularly power-motivated persons might be drawn to physical activity contexts in which they can guide and support others or engage in direct competition with others [10].

Given that individuals with stronger implicit motives consequently have a greater capacity to derive positive feelings from implicit motive satisfaction [8, 13], a positive association between implicit motives and physical activity would be expected if motive-specific incentives are available that initiate the proposed cascade.

Despite a plausible theoretical underpinning, prior reviews neglected implicit motives in the context of physical activity behavior. Yet, the apparent uniqueness of the implicit motive concept also means that implicit motives might have incremental utility for understanding, predicting, or promoting physical activity behavior. For instance, knowing how implicit motives are related to physical activity could inform the design of interventions that increase physical activity by creating opportunities for individuals to satisfy their dominant implicit motives.

In order to provide a systematic overview of the breadth of evidence on the relationship between implicit motives and physical activity, and identify important research gaps that should be addressed in the future [15, 16], we conducted a scoping review of the empirical data linking physical activity and the three major implicit motives. Following a systematic literature search, we aimed to answer the following research questions:

  1. 1.

    What is the relationship between implicit motives and physical activity (based on both observational and intervention studies)?

  2. 2.

    Have studies tried to tailor a physical activity promotion intervention based on persons’ implicit motives (based on intervention studies)?

    • 2.1. If yes, did these tailored interventions lead to larger effects compared to other tailored or non-tailored interventions (based on intervention studies)?

Methods

This scoping review was registered with PROSPERO (CRD42023392198) and its reporting follows the PRISMA extension for scoping reviews (PRISMA-ScR) guideline [17].Footnote 2

A comprehensive literature search was undertaken in the PsycInfo, PubMed, and Web of Science databases. The main search was performed on January, 23 (PubMed, Web of Science) and January, 24 (PsycInfo), 2023, respectively. A supplementary search in the same three databases was performed on August 07, 2024. The search string combined keywords capturing implicit motives and different forms of physical activity. As an example, the search string used in the Web of Science database was: TS = ((implicit AND (motiv* OR achievement OR affiliation OR affiliation-intimacy OR power)) AND (exercis* OR sport$ OR “physical activity”)).

JB conducted the database searches. JB imported the records into a reference manager (main search: Zotero, supplementary search: EndNote) to manage the screening process and remove duplicates. JB screened the titles and abstracts against the eligibility criteria. NS cross-checked a subset of records for eligibility in this stage of the screening process (main search: 200 [26.7%] randomly selected records; supplementary search: 26 [100.0%] records). Discrepancies were resolved by discussion and ineligible texts were discarded. In the next step, the full texts of the remaining English-language publications were screened for eligibility by JB and NS; German-language texts were only screened by JB. Discrepancies were resolved by discussion. Ineligible texts were discarded. The reference lists of eligible publications were hand-searched by JB to find any additional relevant literature; thereby identified full-texts were screened for eligibility by JB.

We included publications if the following inclusion criteria were met: (1) written in English or German and published prior to our search dates; (2) original research published in peer-reviewed scientific journals or booksFootnote 3; (3) observational or intervention study design, whereby intervention studies did not have to be randomized controlled trials (e.g., one-arm pilot interventions were eligible, too); (4) implicit motives measured via the gold standard measurement Picture Story Exercise [18], the Thematic Apperception Test in the tradition of Heckhausen [19], the Implicit Association Test by Slabbinck et al. [20], the Multi Motive Grid [21], or the Operant Motive Test [22] were linked to physical activity behavior (e.g., minutes of physical activity/week).

We excluded publications if at least one of the following exclusion criteria was met: (1) wrong language; (2) wrong document type (e.g., conference papers); (3) implicit motives not measured or assessed with an ineligible method; (4) physical activity not measured or only ineligible aspects of physical activity assessed (e.g., motor performance, intentions); (5) no individual-level data. No restrictions regarding publication date or participant age were applied. Relevant information were extracted from the publications by JB and independently double-checked by NS and MGK. The following information were extracted: (1) authors and publication year; (2) study design; (3) sample characteristics (sample size and if available age and sex); (4) short study summary; (5) methods used to assess implicit motives and physical activity and, if applicable, other relevant psychological concepts; (6) main results, based on reported correlation and/or regression coefficients and comparable results.

In order to also systematically generate insights into the methodological quality of included studies, JB and MGK independently conducted a critical appraisal using an adaptation of the JBI critical appraisal checklist for analytical cross-sectional studies [23]. Subsequently, the two authors formed a consensus rating for each included study by discussing their individual ratings.

We synthesized the evidence qualitatively.

Results

The literature search identified 1047 potentially relevant records. Ultimately, five eligible publications reporting seven unique studies and comprising a total of 550 participants were identified [24,25,26,27,28]. Figure 2 shows a PRISMA flow diagram [29].

Fig. 2
figure 2

PRISMA flow diagram. Note: Depicted are the processes of literature search, screening, and selection using the PRISMA flow diagram template [29]. The asterisk indicates that there may have been multiple reasons for excluding any single publication; here, only one reason is counted per publication. Abbreviations: IMs = implicit motives; PA = physical activity. Figure available at https://osf.io/8jyhg/ [12], under a CC-BY4.0 license

Three publications reported the results of one study each [25,26,27]. Gröpel et al. [24] reported findings from three studies and Wegner et al. [28] reported findings from two studies. However, Study 3 reported by Gröpel et al. [24] and Study 1 reported by Wegner et al. [28] used data from the same sample but focused on different implicit motives.

All eligible studies had an observational design. Four publications [24, 25, 27, 28] reported findings from six cross-sectional studies; one publication [26] reported findings from a prospective longitudinal study. No eligible intervention study was identified (research question 2); thus, research question 2.1 could not be answered.

Table 1 provides a detailed summary of the included publications.

Table 1 Summary of relevant studies

Main findings

Six studies (85.7%) investigated the relationship between the achievement motive and physical activity [24,25,26,27]. Five cross-sectional studies consistently found small-to-large, positive associations between the achievement motive and self-reported sports participation in a university leisure sports program and various sports clubs [24], self-reported exercise behavior [25], and self-reported training hours in gymnasts [27]. Most of these associations also reached statistical significance. The only prospective longitudinal study reported mixed (positive or negligible) associations between the achievement motive and self-reported and actual gym attendance across the study period [26].

Three studies (42.9%) examined the relationship between the affiliation motive and physical activity [25,26,27]. Two cross-sectional studies found statistically non-significant small-to-moderate, positive, and small, negative associations between the affiliation motive and self-reported exercise behavior [25] and self-reported training hours in gymnasts [27], respectively. The longitudinal study by Kopp et al. [26] found no significant associations between the affiliation motive and gym attendance.

Four studies (57.1%) investigated the relationship between the power motive and physical activity [26,27,28]. One cross-sectional study did not find an association between the power motive and self-reported training hours in gymnasts [27]. In two cross-sectional studies, Wegner et al. [28] found that the fear component of the power motive specifically was relatively consistently, positively associated with sports practice workloads in elite tennis players and karateka. Results for the hope component were inconsistent but pointed towards a negative association with physical activity [28]. The longitudinal study by Kopp et al. [26] found no significant associations between the power motive and gym attendance.

Moderator analyses

A narrative review did not reveal a moderating effect of age or sex/gender. However, the associations between the achievement motive and physical activity were particularly consistent and pronounced in athletes vs. non-athletes and in sports-specific vs. non-sports-specific settings.

Critical appraisal

All included publications elicited methodological concerns, to varying degrees. Of concern was a mostly insufficient identification and consideration of potential confounding variables [24, 25, 28]. Furthermore, only in four studies [24,25,26,27], the gold-standard implicit motive assessment method, i.e., the Picture Story Exercise, was used. All seven studies measured physical activity via self-report [24,25,26,27,28], often via self-devised single-item questionnaires. Only one study also electronically measured gym attendance using magnetic check-in cards [26], thereby providing objective, albeit indirect, data on physical activity.

Discussion

The present research is the first scoping review of empirical studies linking implicit motives and physical activity. We identified five eligible publications reporting seven unique observational studies; no intervention study was identified. Achievement motive strength was relatively consistently positively associated with physical activity, especially in athletes and in sports-specific settings. There was less and more mixed evidence on the associations between physical activity and affiliation and power motives. Overall, a paucity of studies — particularly intervention studies — examining the relationship between implicit motives and physical activity, methodological shortcomings, and a focus on sports contexts/athletic populations were identified by this scoping review.

Discussion of the findings

The achievement motive was the most-researched implicit motive. Most studies showed a positive association between physical activity and the achievement motive, i.e., persons with a stronger achievement motive tended to engage in more physical activity. This positive association between achievement motive strength and physical activity is in line with the proposed mechanism of how (the satisfaction of) implicit motives may causally impact physical activity behavior (Fig. 1): Persons with stronger implicit motives can derive stronger positive affect from the satisfaction of their implicit motives [8, 13]. Hence, a positive association between implicit motive strength and physical activity would be expected since stronger positive affect should also translate into a stronger motivation to re-engage in the respective physical activity. However, this only holds true if motive-specific incentives are present and can be attained. Since sports contexts — which were commonly studied — typically provide ample achievement incentives, such as performance feedback and the opportunity to set new personal records [10], a motive-incentive fit is likely. Moreover, through their effects on affect, implicit motives appear to play an important role in the learning of stimuli, behaviors, and contexts that are related to the affective experience [13]. This might further explain these findings.

The positive associations between the achievement motive and physical activity were more consistent and larger in athlete compared to non-athlete samples. Athletes might have learned to associate physical activity with personal standards of excellence, which is the driving force of the achievement motive. This learned association might be weaker in individuals who only participate in physical activity for health reasons.

However, these results should be interpreted with caution due to the methodological limitations of the respective studies, which limit the confidence in the findings to some degree.

Interestingly, in contrast to the achievement motive, there were no consistent (positive) associations between physical activity and the strength of the affiliation and power motives, respectively. It is possible that more research would further clarify the relationship between these implicit motives and physical activity, as only few studies — in part with important methodological limitations — investigated them. Alternatively, it is possible that the study contexts lacked appropriate incentives for affiliation (e.g., cooperative settings) and power (e.g., direct comparisons, guiding others) motives [10].

Only one publication considered the presence of motive-specific incentives [27]. Surprisingly, the authors did not find any evidence that the presence of suitable incentives for the achievement motive (perceived failure, perceived autonomy) and the affiliation motive (training with others vs. alone) moderated the respective relationship with training hours in gymnasts [27].

Limitations and directions for future research

At present, it is not possible to infer causality; future studies will need to better control for confounding and apply other study designs to clearly elucidate the temporal order, distinguish cause and effect, and clarify the potential role of confounders.

To optimally infer the causal effect of implicit motives on physical activity, it would be necessary to randomize implicit motives. However, randomizing implicit motives directly appears infeasible, for example, because it would necessitate highly invasive interventions, such as controlling parental behavior [30]. Moreover, there are still many open questions pertaining to the stability of implicit motives and changes therein [9], rendering the idea of experimentally altering adults’ implicit motives questionable, too. Thus, a promising alternative approach could be to use implicit motives as the personalization target in tailored physical activity promotion interventions. Since people differ in their implicit motives [9], this could be leveraged to deliver intervention content that taps into persons’ dominant implicit motives. Thereby, the cascade proposed in Fig. 1 could be started deliberately, and, consequently, physical activity made more pleasurable. Whether measurable increases in physical activity would be the consequence remains to be determined.

In any case, such a study would hold great value from multiple perspectives. First, it would generate important insights into the practical value of implicit motives. Since a large part of the empirical literature surrounding implicit motives and their relevance for behavioral outcomes is observational in nature [31], a randomized controlled trial could strengthen the available database. As the proposed mechanism depicted in Fig. 1 is consistent with the theory surrounding implicit motives, a randomized controlled trial whose design adheres to this theory could help to further develop the theory surrounding implicit motives — either by supporting current theorizing or by refuting it. Specifically, a randomized controlled trial would help to clarify the temporal order of events (i.e., implicit motives preceding behavior) and eliminate, or at least strongly limit, confounding. In addition, such a study would uniquely add to the field of personalized behavior change interventions — in this case, personalized physical activity promotion [32, 33]. Lastly, if the personalization based on implicit motives turns out to indeed result in clinically meaningful increases in PA, the “toolbox” for physical activity promotion could be expanded.

Furthermore, as mentioned above, methodological limitations limit the confidence in the implications of the current body of evidence. Hence, we believe it to be important that future studies adopt measures that increase their methodological quality. This should include prioritizing standardized versions of the Picture Story Exercise for assessing implicit motives [8, 34]. Similarly, direct and objective measures of physical activity (e.g., accelerometry) should be deployed more, especially when assessing overall physical activity as compared to sports-/exercise-specific physical activity. Alternatively and/or complementarily, validated physical activity questionnaires should be utilized. When conducting further observational studies, it will also be important to put greater emphasis on identifying and controlling for potential confounding variables. Collectively, these measures will increase the methodological quality of future studies, and thus the confidence in their results.

It will also be worthwhile to conduct studies that focus on more diverse, yet specific populations (e.g., different age groups); different types of physical activity (e.g., aerobic vs. muscle-strengthening activities); and on specific implicit motives. Once more studies have been conducted on the relationship between implicit motives and physical activity, it will certainly also be worthwhile to perform a meta-analysis of published results. In the present work, we decided not to perform a meta-analysis because of the small overall body of literature and the pronounced heterogeneity in the methodology of the included studies.

Lastly, we cannot exclude the possibility of publication bias leading, potentially leading to an overestimation of the relationship between implicit motives and physical activity. Future meta-analyses should thus also incorporate an investigation of publication bias (e.g., using funnel plots).

Conclusion

The results of this scoping review suggest that the strength of the implicit achievement motive is positively associated with physical activity, particularly in athletes and in sports-specific settings. This scoping review further identified the need for more and methodologically optimized studies to improve our understanding of the relationship between implicit motives and physical activity, particularly among the general population.

Data availability

Data sharing is not applicable to this article as no datasets were generated or analyzed during the current study. All relevant information are included in the main article and the online supplementary materials, which are available at https://osf.io/8jyhg/ [12].

Notes

  1. The following supplementary information can be found in the Online Supplementary Materials [12]: further details on implicit motives (concept; measurement; distinctions), methodological details (search strategy; critical appraisal; data synthesis), more fine-grained results (summary of an additional publication, which was ineligible but extraordinarily thematically fitting; subgroup and sensitivity analyses; results of the critical appraisal); further points of discussion (randomizing and altering implicit motives); and deviations from the PROSPERO protocol with the according rationale.

  2. Some additional methodological details included in the PRISMA-ScR guideline can be found in the Online Supplementary Materials [12].

  3. We did not search for (publications in) books specifically; however, if we had found an otherwise eligible (publication in a) book via one of the searched databases, we would have included it.

References

  1. Garcia-Hermoso A, Lopez-Gil JF, Ramirez-Velez R, Alonso-Martinez AM, Izquierdo M, Ezzatvar Y. Adherence to aerobic and muscle-strengthening activities guidelines: a systematic review and meta-analysis of 3.3 million participants across 32 countries. Br J Sports Med. 2023;57(4):225–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Katzmarzyk PT, Friedenreich C, Shiroma EJ, Lee IM. Physical inactivity and non-communicable disease burden in low-income, middle-income and high-income countries. Br J Sports Med. 2022;56(2):101–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. McTiernan A, Friedenreich CM, Katzmarzyk PT, Powell KE, Macko R, Buchner D, et al. Physical activity in cancer prevention and survival: a systematic review. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2019;51(6):1252–61.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Bull FC, Al-Ansari SS, Biddle S, Borodulin K, Buman MP, Cardon G, et al. World Health Organization 2020 guidelines on physical activity and sedentary behaviour. Br J Sports Med. 2020;54(24):1451–62.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Santos AC, Willumsen J, Meheus F, Ilbawi A, Bull FC. The cost of inaction on physical inactivity to public health-care systems: a population-attributable fraction analysis. Lancet Glob Health. 2023;11(1):e32–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Ding D, Lawson KD, Kolbe-Alexander TL, Finkelstein EA, Katzmarzyk PT, van Mechelen W, et al. The economic burden of physical inactivity: a global analysis of major non-communicable diseases. Lancet. 2016;388(10051):1311–24.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Gottschalk S, König HH, Weber A, Leitzmann MF, Stein MJ, Peters A, et al. Costs associated with insufficient physical activity in Germany: cross-sectional results from the baseline examination of the German national cohort (NAKO). Eur J Health Econ. 2024. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1007/s10198-024-01697-9. Advance online publication.

  8. Schultheiss OC, Köllner MG. Implicit motives. In: John OP, Robins RW, editors. Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research. 4th ed. Guilford; 2021. p. 385–410.

  9. Denzinger F, Brandstätter V. Stability of and changes in implicit motives. A narrative review of empirical studies. Front Psychol. 2018;9:777.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Wegner M, Brinkmann K. Implicit motives in sport and exercise. In: Schüler J, Wegner M, Plessner H, Eklund RC, editors. Sport and Exercise Psychology: Springer International Publishing; 2023. p. 193–222. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1007/978-3-031-03921-8_9.

  11. Schultheiss OC, Brunstein JC. Assessment of implicit motives with a research version of the TAT: picture profiles, gender differences, and relations to other personality measures. J Pers Assess. 2001;77(1):71–86.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Brummer J, Sudharsanan N, Köllner MG. The relationship between implicit motives and physical activity: a scoping review–Supplementary Materials. https://osf.io/8jyhg/. 2024.

  13. Schultheiss OC, Köllner MG. Implicit motives, affect, and the development of competencies: a virtuous-circle model of motive-driven learning. In: Pekrun R, Linnenbrink-Garcia L, editors. International Handbook of Emotions in Education: Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group; 2014. p. 73–95.

  14. Stevens CJ, Baldwin AS, Bryan AD, Conner M, Rhodes RE, Williams DM. Affective determinants of physical activity: a conceptual framework and narrative review. Front Psychol. 2020;11:568331.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Campbell F, Tricco AC, Munn Z, Pollock D, Saran A, Sutton A, et al. Mapping reviews, scoping reviews, and evidence and gap maps (EGMs): the same but different-the “Big Picture” review family. Syst Rev. 2023;12(1):45.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Munn Z, Pollock D, Khalil H, Alexander L, McLnerney P, Godfrey CM, et al. What are scoping reviews? Providing a formal definition of scoping reviews as a type of evidence synthesis. JBI Evid Synth. 2022;20(4):950–2.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Tricco AC, Lillie E, Zarin W, O’Brien KK, Colquhoun H, Levac D, et al. PRISMA extension for scoping reviews (PRISMA-ScR): checklist and explanation. Ann Intern Med. 2018;169(7):467–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Schönbrodt FD, Hagemeyer B, Brandstatter V, Czikmantori T, Gröpel P, Hennecke M, et al. Measuring implicit motives with the picture story exercise (PSE): databases of expert-coded German stories, pictures, and updated picture norms. J Pers Assess. 2021;103(3):392–405.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Heckhausen H. Hoffnung und Furcht in der Leistungsmotivation [Hope and fear in achievement motivation]: Hain; 1963.

  20. Slabbinck H, De Houwer J, Van Kenhove P. Convergent, discriminant, and incremental validity of the Pictorial Attitude Implicit Association Test and the Picture Story Exercise as measures of the implicit power motive. Eur J Pers. 2013;27(1):30–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Sokolowski K, Schmalt HD, Langens TA, Puca RM. Assessing achievement, affiliation, and power motives all at once: the multi-motive grid (MMG). J Pers Assess. 2000;74(1):126–45.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Kuhl J, Scheffer D. Der operante multi-motive-test (OMT): manual [The operant multi-motive-test (OMT): Manual]. 1999.

  23. Moola S, Munn Z, Tufanaru C, Aromataris E, Sears K, Sfetcu R, et al. Chapter 7: Systematic reviews of etiology and risk. In: Aromataris E, Munn Z, editors. JBI Manual for Evidence Synthesis; 2020. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.46658/JBIMES-20-08.

  24. Gröpel P, Wegner M, Schüler J. Achievement motive and sport participation. Psychol Sport Exerc. 2016;27:93–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Jacob M, Guarnaccia V. Motivational and behavioral correlates of life satisfaction in an elderly sample. Psychol Rep. 1997;80(3 Pt 1):811–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Kopp PM, Senner V, Kehr HM, Gröpel P. Achievement motive, autonomous motivation, and attendance at fitness center: a longitudinal prospective study. Psychol Sport Exerc. 2020;51:101758. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.psychsport.2020.101758.

  27. Schütz LM, Schultheiss OC. Implicit motives, laterality, sports participation and competition in gymnasts. Front Psychol. 2020;11:900.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Wegner M, Wieland A, Mempel G. The implicit fear of power motive is associated with practice time in elite karateka and tennis players. Int J Sport Exerc Psychol. 2017;15(3):258–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Page MJ, McKenzie JE, Bossuyt PM, Boutron I, Hoffmann TC, Mulrow CD, et al. The PRISMA 2020 statement: an updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ. 2021;372: n71.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Köllner MG, Janson KT, Bleck K. The social biopsychology of implicit motive development. In: Schultheiss OC, Mehta PH, editors. Routledge International Handbook of Social Neuroendocrinology. Abingdon: Routledge; 2019. p. 568–85.

    Google Scholar 

  31. Schultheiss OC. Implicit motives. In: John OP, Robins RW, Pervin LA, editors. Handbook of Personality: theory and research. 3rd ed. New York: Guilford; 2008. p. 603–33.

    Google Scholar 

  32. Ma JK, Floegel TA, Li LC, Leese J, De Vera MA, Beauchamp MR, et al. Tailored physical activity behavior change interventions: challenges and opportunities. Transl Behav Med. 2021;11(12):2174–81.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Tong HL, Quiroz JC, Kocaballi AB, Fat SCM, Dao KP, Gehringer H, et al. Personalized mobile technologies for lifestyle behavior change: a systematic review, meta-analysis, and meta-regression. Prev Med. 2021;148:106532.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Schultheiss OC, Pang JS. Measuring implicit motives. In: Robins RW, Fraley RC, Krueger R, editors. Handbook of Research Methods in Personality Psychology: Guilford; 2007. p. 322–44.

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL. This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors. The included publications reported funding from federal agencies, research agencies, universities, and the Templeton Foundation.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

JB, NS, and MGK contributed to the conceptualization, formal analysis, investigation, and visualization steps of this scoping review. JB drafted the manuscript. NS and MGK validated JB’s initial work. JB, NS, and MGK revised the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Julian Brummer.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Not applicable.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Brummer, J., Sudharsanan, N. & Köllner, M.G. The relationship between implicit motives and physical activity: a scoping review. Syst Rev 13, 264 (2024). https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s13643-024-02678-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s13643-024-02678-y

Keywords